Infants who are fed milk based formulas are more likely to prefer flavors at 4 5 years of age

  • View PDF

Infants who are fed milk based formulas are more likely to prefer flavors at 4 5 years of age

Volume 23, Issue 9, 6 May 2013, Pages R401-R408

Infants who are fed milk based formulas are more likely to prefer flavors at 4 5 years of age

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.037Get rights and content

As most parents and caregivers are aware, feeding children a nutritionally balanced diet can be challenging. Children are born with a biological predisposition to prefer sweet and to avoid bitter foods such as green leafy vegetables. It has been hypothesized that this predisposition evolved to attract children to energy-dense foods while discouraging the consumption of toxins. Although this may have enhanced survival in environments historically characterized by food scarcity, it is clearly maladaptive in many of today's food environments where children are surrounded by an abundance of sweet-tasting, unhealthful foods and beverages that place them at risk for excessive weight gain. Because overweight or obese children tend to become overweight or obese adults who are at risk for a range of cardiovascular diseases, it is of primary importance to develop effective evidence-based strategies to promote the development of healthy eating styles. Fortunately, accumulating evidence suggests that, starting before birth and continuing throughout development, there are repeated and varied opportunities for children to learn to enjoy the flavors of healthful foods. Because flavors are transmitted from the maternal diet to amniotic fluid and breast milk, mothers who consume a variety of healthful foods throughout pregnancy and lactation provide their infants with an opportunity to learn to like these flavors. This in turn eases the transition to healthful foods at weaning. In contrast, infants fed formula learn to prefer its invariant flavor profile, which differs from breast milk, and may initially be less accepting of flavors not found in formula. This process can continue throughout weaning and into childhood if infants are repeatedly exposed to a variety of healthful foods, even if they initially dislike them. These early-life sensory experiences establish food preferences and dietary patterns that set the stage for lifelong dietary habits.

© 2017 S. Karger AG, Basel

• Children's dietary habits reflect their basic biology, which predisposes them to prefer sweet tastes and to avoid bitter-tasting foods such as green leafy vegetables. Although once adaptive, in modern food environments children's proclivity for unhealthful foods can place them at risk for obesity and a number of diseases.

• Flavors from the mother's diet are transmitted to amniotic fluid and breast milk, and children have repeated and varied opportunities to learn to like the flavors of healthful foods they will likely encounter during weaning.

• At weaning, 8-10 exposures to a food will increase intake even if the food is initially rejected; further exposures may be required to increase liking. Exposing infants to a variety of flavors promotes infants' willingness to consume novel foods.

Introduction

Parents and caregivers face the ubiquitous challenge of providing their children with a balanced diet that promotes healthy growth and development. The USDA [1] recommends that families meet this challenge by feeding children a diverse, nutrient-dense diet that contains vegetables, fruits, whole grains, low-fat dairy products, and quality protein sources. As many parents lament, meeting these dietary recommendations is difficult for a number of reasons - not the least of which is that children tend to dislike vegetables and prefer sugar-sweetened foods and beverages. As a result, children generally avoid eating most vegetables and forgo consumption of natural sources of sugars such as fruit in favor of foods and beverages that are high in added sugars [2,3]. This preference for simple sugars and energy-dense foods over nutrient-rich alternatives has dire health consequences. Children's poor dietary habits are a risk factor for several diseases, including pediatric obesity, type 2 diabetes, and hypertension, which have traditionally afflicted older adults [4,5].

This article provides an understanding of why children prefer or dislike certain foods and how we can shift their inborn preferences through early sensory experience. It begins with a brief overview of the ontogeny of sweet and bitter taste perception, both of which have important functional significance in children's consumption of healthful, bitter-tasting vegetables and unhealthful, sweet-tasting desserts and beverages. It then reviews how early flavor experiences interact with the plasticity of the chemosensory system to shift children's preferences and acceptance of fruit and vegetables. In sum, this research shows that, although children are born with biological predispositions to prefer highly sweet-tasting foods and beverages over healthful, less sweet alternatives, their preferences can be altered by early experiences from gestation through weaning and do not necessarily determine lifelong dietary habits.

Taste and Flavor Perception

Flavor is a powerful determinant of human consummatory behavior. Although in everyday language the terms flavor and taste are often used interchangeably, flavor refers to the integrated sensation that arises from the combined input of taste, chemosensation, and olfaction [6]. For example, when we consume an orange soda, sugars and citric acid come into contact with taste receptors throughout the oral cavity and the gut, which send messages to the brain that allow us to perceive the sweet and sour taste of this beverage. We also experience the bite of carbon dioxide, which activates trigeminal nerve fibers that innervate the nasal and oral cavity, triggering chemosensation. In addition, the citrus odor travels retronasally along the back of the nasopharynx toward the roof of the nasal cavity, reaching olfactory receptors located in the epithelium of the nasal cavity. Unlike the limited number of primary tastes, which consist of sweet, sour, bitter, savory, and salty, there are thousands of distinctive odors with separable sensations that allow us to experience a rich array of flavors.

Relative to other sensory capacities such as vision and audition, the sense of taste begins to emerge relatively early. Behavioral studies using a variety of techniques suggest that by the last trimester of prenatal development taste buds are capable of detecting and communicating information to central nervous system structures responsible for organizing and controlling affective behaviors (for a more extensive review see [7]). Similarly, the olfactory bulb and receptor cells are functional by the last trimester. Given the extensive prenatal development of the chemosensory system, it is not surprising that the newborn is sensitive and responsive to odor, taste, and flavor stimuli at birth.

Accumulating research suggests that preferences for basic tastes are a major determinant of children's food choices and acceptance patterns [8,9,10,11]. Over the past few decades, our understanding of children's perception and preference for basic tastes has grown substantially (for a review see [7]). We now know that children live in their own sensory world, with their sensitivities and preferences for tastes changing throughout childhood. Responses to basic tastes are remarkably similar across cultures and species, which suggests these responses are a product of children's basic biology.

Sweet Taste

In nature, most sugars (e.g., fructose, maltose, and sucrose) have a small molecular weight and are found primarily in plants. These sugars provide sources of glucose, a key source of energy. For infants, the sweet sugar lactose found in breast milk can also be metabolized to provide glucose energy. Indirect evidence from early studies of fetuses [12], together with findings from studies of premature infants, suggests that detection of sweet taste stimuli is possible during late gestation [13]. This early experience may prepare newborns to detect and accept the basic taste of sweet found in breast milk, which contributes to their survival.

Palatable tastes, such as sucrose, are thought to induce sensory pleasure, which elicits appetitive reactions. As shown in Figure 1, tasting 0.73 M sucrose elicits facial relaxation, sucking movements, and sometimes smiling in newborns, as first described by Steiner [14] and later by Rosenstein and Oster [15]. Consistent with this, research has repeatedly demonstrated that infants preferentially consume sweet-tasting solutions relative to water and can differentiate varying degrees of sweetness (0.05-0.30 M) and different kinds of sugars; sucrose, fructose, glucose, and lactose [16]. These findings converge with physiological findings; for example, administration of drops of aqueous sucrose (0.73 M) to the tongues of newborn infants produced greater relative left-side activation in both frontal and parietal regions of the brain, which is considered to be a reliable indicator of positive emotion [17].

Newborn's orofacial responses to the sweet taste of 0.73 M sucrose (left) and the bitter taste of 0.003 M quinine (right) presented approximately 2 h after birth, before the first postnatal feed. Using a syringe, 0.2 mL of each taste solution at room temperature was presented to the central portion of the dorsal surface of the infant's tongue. Reprinted with permission from [15].

Compared to adults, who on average prefer 0.44 M sucrose, 5- to 10-year-old children have a much sweeter tooth, preferring 0.54 M sucrose [18], almost double the concentration of most soft drinks [19]. The higher preference for sucrose observed throughout childhood may be related to rapid physical growth during this time [20]. This hypothesis is supported by evidence that adolescents with higher sweet preferences also have higher levels of a biomarker for bone growth than do those with lower sweet preferences [21]. Because this biomarker increases during growth spurts, age-related declines in preference for sweet taste may correspond with cessation of physical growth [22,23].

Bitter Taste

In contrast to sweet taste, bitter taste appears to be disliked by infants at birth; as shown in Figure 1, they gape when a bitter quinine solution (0.003 M) or urea solution (0.15-0.25 M) is placed on the tongue [14,15]. However, intake studies reveal that newborns consume similar amounts of 0.18-0.48 M urea in water or in a weak sucrose solution compared to the diluent alone [24,25]. Differential consumption of bitter urea solutions does not occur until infants are approximately 2 weeks of age [26] and is evidenced throughout childhood by a general avoidance of bitter-tasting foods. Together, the orofacial and consumption studies suggest that intake regulation of bitter solutions may mature postnatally.

Recent research in adults and children has focused on understanding the role genes play in individual differences in sensitivity to bitter tastes. Of the 25 human bitter taste receptor genes currently identified, TAS2R38 is the most commonly studied (for a review see [27]). Polymorphisms in the genes that encode this receptor determine much of the variation in taste sensitivity for a class of bitter-tasting chemicals that includes synthetic thiourea compounds (e.g., propylthiouracil [PROP]) and natural plant toxins (e.g., goitrin) found in cruciferous vegetables such as broccoli [28]. Due in part to polymorphisms on the TAS2R38 gene, some individuals have a high sensitivity threshold for this class of bitter tastes, while others have lower thresholds and as a result are more sensitive to the bitterness in cruciferous vegetables [29]. In addition to these individual differences, psychophysical studies have shown that PROP sensitivity appears to decrease with age. Children heterozygous for a TAS2R38 variant perceived lower concentrations of this bitter chemical than did heterozygous adolescents, who in turn detected lower concentrations than heterozygous adults [30,31]. Such sensitivity to this class of bitter tastes may contribute to reduced acceptance of cruciferous vegetables during childhood.

Evolution, Today's Obesogenic Environment, and Sensory Learning: A Bitter-Sweet Story

How do we explain children's biological predispositions to prefer sweet-tasting and dislike bitter-tasting foods, even though they lead to maladaptive outcomes that threaten health? Looking back in our evolutionary history reveals that human's current taste perceptions and preferences have been largely shaped by the ecological niches of our evolutionary ancestors. In order to adapt to specific environments that contain some types of food but not others, our sense of taste has changed and, by extension, so has our genome. Early hominoids used their sense of taste to identify nutritious food items among an expansive dietary repertoire. Preference for sweet tastes is thought to have evolved to attract us to energy-producing sugars that are important for growth and development.

However, eating can be dangerous - many risks are associated with making poor food selections, including the potentially lethal ingestion of harmful parasites, bacteria, and chemicals. As a result, rejection of bitter likely evolved to prevent ingestion of potentially dangerous substances, such as poisons, many of which we perceive as bitter. Although these biological predispositions were at one time adaptive, helping us select nutrients and avoid toxins, today preferences for sugary foods and avoidance of bitter vegetables do not provide an adaptive advantage in environments with easy access to a variety of palatable, energy-dense foods and safe fruits and vegetables.

Over the past century, significant changes in our food environment have occurred, including an increase in the number of fast food restaurants and availability of low-cost, energy-dense food options. These changes have been fueled by marketing strategies that target children's inborn preferences for sweet taste [32]. Children can increase their preference for a food product after only a single exposure to a commercial, and this is strengthened with repeated exposures. In turn, these preferences affect their product purchase requests, which ultimately influence their parents' purchasing decisions [33].

The marketing influence on children's food preferences is of particular concern for a number of reasons. First, as discussed above, we do not need to encourage consumption of unhealthful foods, given that children are already predisposed to preferentially consume them. Not only are children attracted to the sweet taste of sugar in these foods, but the presence of sugar can also effectively suppress or mask the bitterness [34,35] that is inherent in some foods and beverages (e.g., caffeinated energy drinks) that children would otherwise avoid.

Second, through familiarization with sweetened versions of foods and beverages that are not inherently sweet-tasting, such as yogurt, milk, or cereal, children develop an expectation that foods should taste sweet [36]. As a result of the intrinsic plasticity of the taste system, preferences for sweeter-tasting foods and beverages are readily acquired through early exposure to sweet taste. For example, longitudinal studies have shown that newborn infants who were regularly fed sugar water preferred significantly higher concentrations of sucrose solutions 2 years later compared to those who had no such experience [37]. Although correlational, these studies suggest that early dietary exposure to sweet foods is associated with later enhanced acceptance of sweet tastes. However, the opposite is also true: just as children's preferences for sweet-tasting foods can be strengthened, preferences for healthful foods can increase as a result of early exposure to the flavors of these foods.

Accumulating evidence suggests that, starting before birth and continuing throughout development, repeated and varied opportunities to learn about the flavors of healthful foods increase later acceptance and consumption of these foods [38]. During fetal development, the intrauterine environment is rich in odor volatiles (i.e., flavors) that change as a function of the mothers' diet. As discussed above, it is likely that the fetus is sensitive to and learning about this ever-changing flavor profile; by the last trimester the taste and olfactory receptors are functional, and the fetus is actively swallowing between 500 and 1,000 mL of amniotic fluid each day [39]. After birth, infants are exposed to a diet that is typically solely milk-based, consisting of breast milk, artificial milk (formula), or both. While the flavor of breast milk, like amniotic fluid, reflects the mother's diet, the invariant flavor profile of formula does not (Fig. 2).

Flavor experiences during pre- and postnatal development. Common flavors are initially experienced during the fetal period in utero, during postnatal feeding, and during weaning. After birth, the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends feeding only breast milk for the first 6 months of life, followed by a combination of solid foods and breast milk until the infant is at least 1 year old. This gradual transition to a diet consisting primarily of solid foods is represented by the gradated borders in the figure. Breastmilk serves to bridge the flavor experiences during the fetal period to those at weaning (represented by solid black arrows). Many mothers choose to feed their infants formula exclusively, or to feed a combination of breastmilk and formula (as represented by the dotted double arrow). In contrast to the varied sensory experiences of breastmilk, the flavor of formula is monotone and lacks the volatiles of the foods of the mother's diet. These experiences nevertheless affect infants' acceptance of similar flavors at weaning (as represented by the solid grey arrow).

Amniotic Fluid and Breast Milk

A wide variety of flavors ingested by the mother are transmitted to amniotic fluid during pregnancy and to breast milk during lactation, including garlic [40], vanilla [41], anise [42], and carrot [43]. This ongoing exposure to the flavors within amniotic fluid and breast milk biases the infant's flavor preferences for these foods. In one study, mothers consumed carrot juice either for 3 consecutive weeks during the last trimester of pregnancy, or during lactation, while those in a control group drank water during pregnancy and lactation and avoided carrots [43]. Infants exposed to the target flavor, either prenatally or postnatally, preferred carrot-flavored relative to plain cereal, whereas the control group showed no such preference. Further work in non-human animals has replicated these effects and has additionally shown that dogs exposed to aniseed throughout the perinatal period (i.e., pre- and postnatal exposure) displayed greater flavor preferences for aniseed at weaning than those exposed to aniseed either pre- or postnatally [44].

These results support the contention that the continuity of flavor experiences provided by breastfeeding helps with the transition to solid foods at weaning. This is further supported by findings that breastfed infants are more accepting of fruit than are formula-fed infants, but only if their mothers regularly ate this food during lactation [45]. Breastfed children may also be more willing to accept novel foods [46] and less picky as they grow older [47,48], especially if their mothers eat a varied diet, which provides a more varied flavor profile in breast milk.

Formula

Infants who are exclusively formula-fed often receive just one type of formula, which limits their exposure to varied flavor experiences [49]. Despite the lack of flavor variety, different types and brands of formulas vary in their characteristic flavor profile, due to differences in their composition and processing [50]. For example, cow-milk-based formulas (CMFs) are described as having low levels of sweetness, with sour and cereal-like characteristics, whereas soy-protein-based formulas have sweet, sour, and bitter tastes. Extensively hydrolyzed protein formulas (ePHFs), the feeding regimen of choice for formula-fed infants who cannot tolerate intact proteins, have high levels of free amino acids, because its proteins are broken down by enzymes. These free amino acids impart a savory, bitter, and sour taste profile, as well as unpleasant odor volatiles (e.g., sulfur volatiles that are found in cruciferous vegetables, such as broccoli [51,52]). Yet, infants fed ePHF early in life readily learn to accept its “off” flavors [53].

By taking advantage of the inherent differences in the flavor profiles among these formulas, researchers have shown that infants develop flavor preferences that reflect the type of formula they are fed. Compared to infants fed CMF, those fed ePHF ate more savory-, sour-, and bitter-tasting infant cereals at a faster rate and showed fewer facial expressions of distaste during feeding [54]. Moreover, ePHF-fed as well as breastfed infants were more likely to display positive facial reactions to savory-tasting cereal, perhaps because breast milk [55] and ePHF [56] are both high in the savory amino acid glutamate. Other research has shown that the length of time infants are fed ePHF influences their responses to savory food; those fed ePHF for at least 3 months showed greater acceptance of a savory broth relative to a plain broth [57]. Evidence shows that these early preferences can be long-lasting. Several years after the last formula exposure, children fed ePHF during infancy were more likely to prefer a sour-tasting apple juice than were children fed CMF [58]. They were also more likely to preferentially consume broccoli, which has flavor notes similar to ePHF [58]. In combination, these studies reveal that the tastes to which infants are exposed during formula feedings affect their acceptance of foods at weaning. However, if these flavors are not part of the family's diet, infants may not reap the benefits of this early sensory learning. Rather, preferences for the foods that the family eats will be acquired at weaning through repeated exposure.

Complementary Feeding: Increasing Preferences for Fruit and Vegetables

At weaning, children transition from a milk-based to a mixed diet that consists of breast milk or formula and a variety of complementary foods. With the milk-based diet, infants learn to prefer foods through repeated exposure (with formula) or through exposure to a variety of flavors (with breast milk). The same is true for children at weaning: converging evidence from several experimental studies indicates that children require 8-10 exposures to the taste of a food in order to increase acceptance of it. It is important to note, however, that increased intake does not always coincide with increased liking[45]. Even when infants begin to consume more of a food after repeated exposure, they may continue to display negative facial responses (such as squinting) while eating it. Because these negative orofacial responses persist after increased acceptance, parents are less likely to offer the food again [59]. To produce shifts in liking that mirror the changes in intake, exposure needs to continue beyond acceptance until liking occurs (i.e., when the infant begins to show fewer facial expressions of distaste) [45,60].

Relative to merely repeating exposure to the same food, exposing infants to a variety of flavors has the added advantage of promoting infants' willingness to consume novel foods. As shown in Figure 3, 8 days of exposure to a variety of vegetables increases acceptance of a novel-tasting vegetable [61,62] but not of a novel fruit [61]. Similarly, 8 days of exposure to a variety of fruits increases acceptance of a novel fruit but not of a novel green vegetable [61]. It appears that the variety of foods presented must share some flavor characteristics of the novel food in order to increase its acceptance. More recent research has shown that exposure to flavor variety continues to be effective in increasing acceptance of fruit between 4 and 8 years of age [63]. However, this study did not find a similar increase in children's preferences for vegetables, which suggests that shifting older children's preference for vegetables may require other strategies such as associative conditioning [64].

Infants' intake of green beans (a) and pears (b) before (white bars) and after (black bars) an 8-day home exposure period in which they were fed green beans, a variety of orange and green vegetables, a variety of fruit, or pears. Infants increased their intake of green beans if they were fed green beans or a variety of vegetables, but not fruit, at home. They also increased their intake of pears if they had been fed pears or a variety of fruit at home. This suggests that, for flavor variety to increase consumption of a novel vegetable, there must be some overlap between the flavor profiles of the variety of foods fed and the novel food [61]. * Significant difference at p < 0.05. + Significant difference at p < 0.08.

It appears that additional factors may moderate the ease with which infants acquire flavor preferences for healthful foods, such as personal characteristics of the child. For example, children who were high in approach temperament were less likely to express facial expressions of distaste (i.e., gape) and consumed more of a bitter green vegetable [65]. Infants with approach temperaments may be more likely to try a greater variety of fruits and vegetables before the onset of neophobia at around 2 years of age. As discussed above, we are also gaining a better understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying individual differences in taste sensitivity. For example, because of genotype differences, some individuals are more sensitive to the bitter taste of some vegetables and as a result may be likely to eat these foods (e.g., [29,66]). Yet another factor shown to be important is the parents' feeding style. Osborne and Forestell [63] found that when children were exposed to a variety of fruits and vegetables, they were less likely to develop a preference for a novel fruit when their mothers reported pressuring them to eat. Thus, it appears that early and repeated sensory experiences, child temperament, taste receptor genotype, as well as the quality of mother-child interactions during feeding are just a few of the factors that interact to determine food preferences during childhood.

Final Remarks

While no single factor is responsible for the dramatic increases in overweight and obesity in the US over the past century, it is generally accepted that the consumption of sugar-sweetened products, especially beverages, is causally linked to increases in risk of chronic diseases, including type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, hypertension, and stroke [67]. This is concerning because children are born with biological predispositions to preferentially consume sweet-tasting foods and beverages instead of other more healthful foods, such as green vegetables.

Whether this early proclivity for sweet tastes leads to later unhealthy dietary habits depends in part on the child's early sensory experiences. Health care providers should encourage pregnant and nursing women to consume healthful diets with a variety of flavors. Infants who are formula-fed should be exposed to a variety of flavors, particularly those associated with fruits and vegetables, while the mother is pregnant and again at weaning. Although we cannot completely change children's innate liking of sweets and disliking of bitterness, we have learned that early sensory experiences, which begin with the flavors of foods the mother eats during pregnancy and lactation, can shape and modify early flavor and food preferences, thereby increasing infants' acceptance of the foods available in their environment. Thus, a mother's healthy diet increases the likelihood that her child will prefer these same healthy foods. Repeated exposure to healthy foods at weaning will maintain and expand these preferences. Infants' healthy dietary repertoire will continue to grow if they are exposed to a variety of healthy foods at weaning and throughout childhood.

To be sure, whether children have the opportunity to learn about healthful flavors early in life depends on many factors. A family's decisions about food purchases and consumption are influenced by a range of socioenvironmental factors, such as culture, financial status, and education (e.g., [68]). An appreciation and a greater understanding of the complexity underlying food choices in families and how these affect the development of children's food preferences will aid in the development of evidence-based strategies and programs to facilitate children's early acceptance of fruit and vegetables.

Disclosure Statement

The author declares that no financial or other conflict of interest exists in relation to the contents of the paper. The writing of this article was supported by Nestlé Nutrition Institute.

  1. US Department of Health and Human Services and US Department of Agriculture: 2015-2020 Dietary Guidelines for Americans, ed 8, 2015. http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/guidelines.
  2. Siega-Riz AM, Deming DM, Reidy KC, Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR: Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: where are we now? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:S38-S51.
  3. Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR, Reidy KC, Deming DM: Food consumption patterns of young preschoolers: Are they starting off on the right path? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110: S52-S59.
  4. Welsh JA, Sharma A, Cunningham SA, Vos MB: Consumption of added sugars and indicators of cardiovascular disease risk among US adolescents. Circulation 2011;123:249-257.
  5. Hartley L, et al: Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables for the primary prevention of cardiovascular diseases. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2013;6:CD009874.
  6. Delwiche JF: The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor. Food Qual Pref 2004;15:137-146.
  7. Forestell CA, Mennella JA: The ontogeny of taste perception and preference throughout childhood; in Doty RL (ed): Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation, ed 3. Hoboken, John Wiley and Sons, 2015, pp 795-830.
  8. Birch LL: Development of food acceptance patterns in the first year of life. Proc Nutr Soc 1998;57:617-624.
  9. Liem DG, Mennella JA: Sweet and sour preferences during childhood: role of early experiences. Dev Psychobiol 2002;41:388-395.
  10. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Reed DR: Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics 2005;115:e216-e222.
  11. Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Reed DR: The proof is in the pudding: children prefer lower fat but higher sugar than do mothers. Int J Obes (Lond) 2012;36:1285-1291.
  12. Liley AW: Disorders of amniotic fluid; in Assali NS (ed): Pathophysiology of Gestation: Fetal Placental Disorders. New York, Academic Press, 1972, pp 157-206.
  13. Maone TR, Mattes RD, Bernbaum JC, Beauchamp GK: A new method for delivering a taste without fluids to preterm and term infants. Dev Psychobiol 1990;13:179-191.
  14. Steiner JE: The gustofacial response: observation on normal and anencephalic newborn infants; in Bosma JF (ed): Fourth Symposium on Oral Sensation and Perception. Bethesda, US Department of Health, Education and Welfare 1973.
  15. Rosenstein D, Oster H: Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev 1988;59:1555-1568.
  16. Desor J, Maller O, Turner RE: Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1973;84:496-501.
  17. Fox NA, Davidson RJ: Taste-elicited changes in facial signs of emotion and the asymmetry of brain electrical activity in human newborns. Neuropsychologia 1986;24:417-422.
  18. Mennella JA, Lukasewycz LD, Griffith JW, Beauchamp GK: Evaluation of the Monell forced-choice, paired-comparison tracking procedure for determining sweet taste preferences across the life span. Chem Senses 2011;36:345-355.
  19. Ventura EE, Davis JN, Goran MI: Sugar content of popular sweetened beverages based on objective laboratory analysis: focus on fructose content. Obesity 2011;19:868-874.
  20. Drewnowski A: Sensory control of energy density at different life stages. Proc Nutr Soc 2000;59:239-244.
  21. Yang L, Grey V: Pediatric reference intervals for bone markers. Clin Biochem 2006;39:561-568.
  22. Coldwell SE, Oswald TK, Reed DR: A marker of growth differs between adolescents with high vs. low sugar preference. Physiol Behav 2009;96:574-580.
  23. Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Lipchock SV, Hwang LD, Reed DR: Preferences for salty and sweet tastes are elevated and related to each other during childhood. PLoS One 2014;9:e92201.
  24. Maller O, Desor JA: Effect of taste on ingestion by human newborns. Symp Oral Sens Percept 1973;4:279-291.
  25. Desor JA, Maller O, Andrews K: Ingestive responses of human newborns to salty, sour, and bitter stimuli. J Comp Physiol Psych 1975;89:966-970.
  26. Kajiura H, Cowart BJ, Beauchamp GK: Early developmental change in bitter taste responses in human infants. Dev Psychobiol 1992;25:375-386.
  27. Hayes JE, Feeney EL, Allen AL: Do polymorphisms in chemosensory genes matter for human ingestive behavior? Food Qual Prefer 2013;30:202-216.
  28. Lipchock SV, Mennella JA, Spielman AJ, Reed DR: Human bitter perception correlates with bitter receptor mRNA expression in taste cells. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:1136-1143.
  29. Sandell MA, Breslin PAS: Variability in a taste-receptor gene determines whether we taste toxins in food. Curr Biol 2006;16:R792-R794.
  30. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Duke FF, Reed DR: Age modifies the genotype-phenotype relationship for the bitter receptor TAS2R38. BMC Genet 2010;11:60.
  31. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Duke FF, Reed DR: Psychophysical dissection of genotype effects on human bitter perception. Chem Senses 2011;36:161-167.
  32. Boyland EJ, Halford JCG: Television advertising and branding: effects on eating behaviour and food preferences in children. Appetite 2013;62:236-41.
  33. American Psychological Association: Report of the APA Task Force on Advertising and Children. Washington, 2004. http://www.apa.org/pubs/info/reports/advertising-children.aspx.
  34. Walters DE: How are bitter and sweet tastes related? Trends Food Sci Tech 1996;7:399-403.
  35. Mennella JA, Reed DR, Mathew PS, Roberts KM, Mansfield CJ: A spoonful of sugar helps the medicine go down: bitter masking by sucrose among children and adults. Chem Senses 2015;40:17-25.
  36. Sullivan SA, Birch LL: Pass the sugar, pass the salt: experience dictates preference. Dev Psychol 1990;26:546.
  37. Beauchamp GK, Moran M: Acceptance of sweet and salty tastes in 2-year-old children. Appetite 1984;5:291-305.
  38. Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA: Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior. J Pediatr Gastr Nutr 2009;48:S25-S30.
  39. Ross MG, Nijland MJ: Fetal swallowing: relation to amniotic fluid regulation. Clin Obstet Gynecol 1997;40:352-365.
  40. Mennella JA, Johnson A, Beauchamp GK: Garlic ingestion by pregnant women alters the odor of amniotic fluid. Chem Senses 1995;20:207-209.
  41. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK: The human infants' response to vanilla flavors in mother's milk and formula. Infant Behav Dev 1996;19:13-19.
  42. Schaal B, Marlier L, Soussignan R: Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother's diet. Chem Senses 2000;25:729-737.
  43. Mennella JA, Jagnow CP, Beauchamp GK: Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics 2001;107:e88.
  44. Hepper PG, Wells DL: Perinatal olfactory learning in the domestic dog. Chem Senses 2006;31:207-212.
  45. Forestell CA, Mennella JA: Early determinants of fruit and vegetable acceptance. Pediatrics 2007;120:1247-1254.
  46. Hausner H, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S, Mølgaard C, Møller P: Breastfeeding facilitates acceptance of a novel dietary flavour compound. Clin Nutr 2010;29:e231-e238.
  47. Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL: Predictors and consequences of food neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:692-698.
  48. Maier-North A, Schaal B, Leathwood P, Issanchou S: The lasting influences of early food-related variety experience: a longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations. PLoS One 2016;11:e0151356.
  49. Nevo N, Rubin L, Tamir A, Levine A, Shaoul R: Infant feeding patterns in the first 6 months: an assessment in full-term infants. J Pediatr Gastr Nutr 2007;45:234-239.
  50. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK: Exploring the beginning of flavor preferences. Chem Senses 2005;30:i242-i243.
  51. Cook DA, Sarett HP: Design of infant formulas for meeting normal and special need; in Lifshitz F (ed): Pediatric Nutrition: Infant Feeding, Deficiencies, Disease. New York, Marcel Dekker, 1982.
  52. Lee YH: Food-processing approaches to altering allergenic potential of milk-based formula. J Pediatr 1992;121:S47-S50.
  53. Mennella JA, Griffin CE, Beauchamp GK: Flavor programming during infancy. Pediatrics 2004;113:840-845.
  54. Mennella JA, Forestell CA, Morgan LK, Beauchamp GK: Early milk feeding influences taste acceptance and liking during infancy. Am J Clin Nutr 2009;90:780S-788S.
  55. Agostoni C, Carratu B, Boniglia C, Riva E, Sanzini E: Free amino acid content in standard infant formulas: comparison with human milk. J Am Coll Nutr 2000;19:434-438.
  56. Ventura AK, San Gabriel A, Hirota M, Mennella JA: Free amino acid content in infant formulas. Nutr Food Sci 2012;42:271-278.
  57. Mennella JA, Castor SM: Sensitive period in flavor learning: effects of duration of exposure to formula flavors on food likes during infancy. Clin Nutr 2012;31:1022-1025.
  58. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK: Flavor experiences during formula feeding are related to preferences during childhood. Early Hum Dev 2002;68:71-82.
  59. Carruth BR, Ziegler PJA, Barr GSI: Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers' decisions about offering a new food. J Am Diet Assoc 2004;104:S57-S64.
  60. Sullivan SA, Birch LL: Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics 1994;93:271-277.
  61. Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM: Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 2008;94:29-38.
  62. Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA: Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:1080-1085.
  63. Osborne CL, Forestell CA: Increasing children's consumption of fruit and vegetables: does the type of exposure matter? Physiol Behav 2012;106:362-368.
  64. Capaldi-Phillips ED, Wadhera D: Associative conditioning can increase liking for and consumption of brussels sprouts in children aged 3 to 5 years. J Acad Nutr Diet 2014;114:1236-1241.
  65. Forestell CA, Mennella JA: More than just a pretty face. The relationship between infant's temperament, food acceptance, and mothers' perceptions of their enjoyment of food. Appetite 2012;58:1136-1142.
  66. Bell KI, Tepper BJ: Short-term vegetable intake by young children classified by 6-n-propylthoiuracil bitter-taste phenotype. Am J Clin Nutr 2006;84:245-251.
  67. Hu FB: Resolved: there is sufficient scientific evidence that decreasing sugar-sweetened beverage consumption will reduce the prevalence of obesity and obesity-related diseases. Obes Rev 2013;14:606-619.
  68. Forestell CA, Dallaire DH: Pregnant behind bars: meeting the nutrition needs of incarcerated pregnant women; in Lammi-Keefe CJ, Couch SC, Philipson E (eds): Handbook of Nutrition and Pregnancy, ed 2. Totowa, Humana Press, pp 55-64.

Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.

Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.